Background
Insurance
is a business, an exercise in statistics, and a game of
psychology between players, all at the same time. The academic literature about
insurance covers topics ranging from the statistical analysis of norms to the
selling of policies, and of course to the fulfillment of obligations. When
people buy insurance, they do so in hopes of never having to use it, and are at
the mercy of two competing motives, viz., best coverage and lowest cost. When
the customer is knowledgeable about the topic, or even thinks beyond the
momentary transaction, the issue comes up as to the nature of the interaction
with the insurance company should there
arise a problem for which the insurance was originally purchased.
There have been some papers on the psychology of home construction insurance [1,2]. The most instructive information about home contractor insurance comes from the Internet, specifically from organization specializing in contractor insurance or specific brokers. Here are three examples, phrased clearly to alert readers to the need for such insurance. Note that these are written from the point of view of the contractor, who must carry the insurance, and NOT from the point of view of insuring the homeowner. Insurance.com asked 1,000 people about their home improvement projects to see whether they were a success or failure. Findings reveal that going over budget and not completing the work are the top renovation fails. Of those who had a home renovation fail: 41% spent more than expected, 39% didn’t finish an important project, 12% had arguments with their partner or spouse as a result of the renovation, 5% experienced fire, flooding or other damage due to the work, 2% damaged a neighbor’s property
https://www.insurance.com/home-and-renters-insurance/contractor-renovation-remodel. You should
verify your contractor’s insurance coverage before hiring him or her by asking
to review a copy of the contractor’s policies. It should include both a
commercial business/general liability insurance policy and workers
compensation. The latter is important because without it, workers remodeling
your home could sue you if injured. Though your liability insurance would pay
for that, up to your limits, it’s best to avoid the situation altogether. https://www.insurance.com/home-and-renters-insurance/contractor-renovation-remodel General
liability insurance policies will usually cover a broad range of damages,
including:
· Faulty
workmanship
· Job-related
injury
· Advertising
injury/defamation
Contractors
or developers may actually be required to have a minimum level of liability
insurance either by law in some states or to win certain contracts that require
it. Companies who complete many design-build projects will definitely want to
have liability insurance in case they are sued for mistakes. Also,
subcontractors are frequently required to carry liability insurance in order to
work for certain general contractors. https://constructioncoverage.com/construction-insurance#types.
The
academic literature of insurances deals with topics which are in the domain of
specialists, such as the way the customers decide about insurance, legal
issues, and other serious topics [2-5]. The subjective aspects, especially
through the theory of reasoned action, tend to be bland, without feeling,
almost as if they were presented from 30,000 feet, without the granularity of
everyday life
[6-8].
There
is a need in the academic literature for studies which deal with the emotions
of people considering insurance. The topic of insurance as an emotional issue
is not unusual because it is the emotion, the anticipation of negative results
from one’s action, or a negative situation in one’s life, which drives the
purchase of insurance. There is the tendency to delay, to rationalize,
tendencies that must be overcome through marketing and sales. Those who are
trying to sell insurance are not interested in theory, but rather in the
correct messages which ‘sell’ the insurance.
For
the business person, the issue is what should be offered, and what should be
communicated about what is being offered. There is no lack of information about
what should be offered; one need only read advertisements for insurance to
understand the depth of insight into the sensitivities and soft spots of
insurance buyers, possessed by those who sell insurance. The goal of this paper
is to apply the rigorous study of communication to the offerings of an
insurance company wishing to sell contractor
insurance.
The objective is to begin a series of investigations into the nature of the
messaging of insurance from the point of view of how a typical respondent
‘feels’.
Method
The
process to understand the mind of the prospective insurance buyer followed the
steps of Mind Genomics, a newly
emerging science of the ‘mind of the everyday’ [9-11]. Mind Genomics explores
the mind of the everyday by presenting respondents with vignettes, combinations
of elements (messages, ideas), obtaining ratings, deconstructing the ratings
into the contributions of the separate elements using regression, and by so
doing revealing the mind of the respondent, standing in for the prospective
insurance purchaser.
Step 1: Raw
Material
Mind
Genomics works at the granular level, with test stimuli drawn from everyday life. Step 1 is
Socratic, beginning with a set of related questions which ‘tell a story’, and
then generating four answers to each question, representing alternatives. The
exercise for this study generated the questions and answers shown in Table 1. These do not, in any fashion,
reflect the full gamut of possible questions and answers in insurance, but
rather represent a tractable set of ideas. An ingoing world-view in Mind
Genomics is that it is better to iterate quickly and inexpensively to find the
answer, rather than to create a possibly ponderous study which might implode
from its own gravitas.
Table 1: The four
questions, and the four answers (elements) for each question.
In
Mind Genomics experiments, the objective is to quickly and inexpensively
identify strong-performing elements, and when necessary move on to the next
iteration. The choice of four questions, each with four alternative answers,
the elements, is based upon the observation that larger numbers of questions
and answers at first are thought to generate a ‘better experiment’ because of
more coverage, but become self-defeating as the effort begins to be
overwhelming to fill the available slots for elements.
Quite
often the larger-scale studies implode, brought to an untimely, early end by
the inability of the group to come to a resolution. This is akin to the
increasingly observed ‘paradox of choice’, where decision-making becomes hard,
even onerous when the number of possible selections increases [12]. The 4x4
design is a compromise, providing enough variation in stimulus set, but small
enough to be executed quickly, and not to be perceived as the one study which
will answer everything.
Step 2:
Vignettes (Combinations of Elements)
The
respondent does not evaluate single elements, the usual process in survey
research. Rather, the study is conducted in the form of an experiment, one
conducted on the computer, with the respondents evaluating combinations of
elements which represent different sets of propositions, or vignettes. The vignettes
comprise combinations of the answers, the elements (see Table 1), without,
however, the question being present. That is, each vignette presents a simple
set of elements without any attempt to connect the answers to create a coherent
but often densely worded paragraph. The experimental design combines these 16
elements into small vignettes, each vignette comprising from two to four
elements, at most one element or answer from a question. The experimental
design ensures that the elements are statistically independent of each other,
allowing the data emerging from the study to be analyzed by OLS (Ordinary
Least-Squares) regression, either at the individual respondent level or at the
group level.
Each
respondent evaluated a unique set of 24 vignettes developed according to a
permutation scheme which maintained the mathematical integrity of the
experimental design, but at the same time ensured that the research covered a
great number of combinations. The permutations ensure that the Mind Genomics
experiment assesses a great number of combinations, 2400 in the case of the 100
respondents in this study. The objective is to cover a great deal of the
underlying design space. Each individual measurement is ‘noisy’ since it is
measured one time. The rationale is that by covering a great deal of the design
space through the permutation strategy, one will obtain a clearer, less-error
prone estimate of the contribution of the individual elements to the rating.
This strategy stands in opposition to the typical strategy of reducing error by
replicating the stimulus many times, thus getting a better estimate of the
measure of central tendency, the mean.
The
strategy of permuting the combinations is analogous to the strategy behind the
MRI, which takes many ‘pictures’ of the same tissue from different angles, and
then produces a better, composite, through subsequent computer recreation of
the tissue from the different pictures, at different angles [13]. The objective
of Mind Genomics is to determine how the respondent weights the different
elements to arrive at a decision. Thus, the vignettes are only vehicles to
embed the elements, and to present these elements in a way which forces the
respondent to assign a ‘gut feeling response’ to the combination. The creation
of 24 combinations prevents the respondent from assigning the ‘politically
correct answer’ or from ‘gaming the system’.
Indeed,
often the comment from a respondent is of the order of ‘I could not figure out
what the right answer was…so I guessed’.
Step 3: Steps at the Start of the Start of the Evaluation
The
respondents are invited to participated by an email. The on-line panel
provider, Luc.id Inc., maintains groups of respondents around the world in more
than 40 countries. The respondents have already agreed to participate, and are
accustomed to doing surveys. Whereas in previous years, at least until the
advent of the Internet, respondents could be found who had NOT participated
during the three months prior to the study, today’s world comprises very few of
these ‘naïve’ panelists, without experience. Although the respondents from
Luc.id can be said to be ‘experienced’, they reflect the typical respondents
available today, consumers, not experts. At the beginning of the interview the
respondent provided information about age, gender and answer a third
classification question. How do you feel about insurance companies? 1=I trust
them, 2=I have to watch them, 3=I don't trust them, 4=Not applicable.
Step 4: Evaluation
of Vignettes
The
respondent read the following instructions: Here is the description of a
situation about a contractor and insurance. How would you feel about this
situation if this were you?
· Definitely avoid
this
· Move forward
slowly with trepidation
· Move forward
quickly with trepidation
· Move forward
quickly
· No hesitation
The respondents then read each of the 24 vignettes, doing so fairly quickly (generally less than 5-6 seconds for each vignette). In general, Mind Genomics studies are executed fairly quickly on the Internet, especially when the topic is simple. The entire study lasted about 3-4 minutes for each respondent. The respondents are not deeply interested in the topic, and there is no way for the researcher to subtly influence the respondent regarding either the seriousness of the topic or the expected ‘right answer.’ Thus, the answers represent the intuitive best guess from the respondents, who are both uncertain about what is correct, but motivated to finish the study with some sense of honesty because they belong to a panel of respondents who do many studies.
Step 5:
Transformation of the Ratings
Managers
prefer simple information, such as ‘yes/no’ rather than scalar information. Indeed,
the scalar information, while capturing nuances of feeling, is hard to
understand. When managers are presented with the results, average ratings on a
5-point scale, for example, often the first question is not about the data
themselves in terms of the answers to questions, but rather the more basic
question ‘what does a 3 mean?’, and so forth.
In
light of this apparent uncertainty in the interpretation of questions, consumer
researchers as well as political pollsters have opted to present their data in
binary terms when talking to the public, although they use the metric or scalar
data for many of their statistical
computations
in the background, for other purposes. In the spirit of this effort to make the
data simpler to understand we transform the ratings to a binary scale. Ratings
of 1-3 are recoded to ‘0’; ratings of 4-5 are recoded to ‘100’. To each recoded
response we add a very small random number (<10-5), in order to add small
but necessary variation to the ratings, for subsequent analyses by OLS (Ordinary
Least-Squares Regression).
Step 6: Mind-Set
Segmentation
The
objective here is to move beyond conventional division of people into age,
gender, and even attitude towards insurance companies. Rather, the objective is
to divide people by how they respond to messages about a micro-topic, in this
case the insurance to be purchase by the homeowner for contractor failure. There
would be no other way to divide people by mind-sets, other than ‘doing the Mind Genomics
experiment
and dividing the respondents based on the data from that specific experiment’. The
process is straightforward. The data allow us to generate an Generate
individual-level model (equation) for each respondent, relating the
presence/absence of the 16 elements to the binary transformed ratings for that
individual respondent, and then cluster (segment) the respondents into two and
three groups (mind-sets) based upon the pattern of the 16 coefficients for each
respondent. Recall that the 24 vignettes for each respondent were created by an
underlying
experimental design.
That designed produced 24 unique vignettes, unlike the 24 vignettes for any
other respondent. Each respondent thus generates a set of 24 rows of data, with
the first 16 columns of data being the 16 elements, with the cells having
either a 0 when the element is absent from the particular vignette, and present
when the element is present in the vignette. The 17th column is the
binary transformed rating.
The
data for each respondent are subject to OLS (ordinary least-squares)
regression. The independent variables are the 16 elements, the dependent
variable is the binary rating. The result equation, calculated at the
respondent level is: Binary Transform=k0 + k1(A1) + k2(A2)
… k16(D4). The data matrix now comprises 100 rows of coefficients
k1…k16. The additive constant is ignored. The coefficients give a sense of the
driving value of the element towards rating 4-5 (4=move forward quickly; 5=no
hesitation). The respondents are clustered using k-means clustering [14].
The
respondents are objects to be put together in homogeneous groups, based upon
the pattern of coefficients. The criterion is the quantity (1-Pearson
Correlation (R)). The quantity (1-R) takes on the value 0 when two respondents
show a perfectly linear correlation of +1, based on their 16 coefficients,
meaning that they virtually identical in the criteria of judgment. They belong
in the same cluster or mind-set. Two people belong in different mind-sets when
the quantity (1-R) takes on the value 2, which occurs when the coefficients of
the two respondents move in precisely opposite directions. These two
respondents belong in different clusters, or mind-sets.
Step 7: Create
‘GRAND’ Models (Equations) for Each Key Subgroup, Relating the Presence/Absence
of Elements to the Transformed Rating
The
model is expressed in the same format as the model for the individual
respondent, except that the model is created using ALL the data from a
particular group (age, gender, mind-set, and opinion of contractors). The
equation once again is: Binary Transform=k0 + k1(A1) + k2(A2)
… k16(D4).
Step 8: Create
Models (Equations) for Response Time, for Each Subgroup
The Mind Genomics program measures the response time, defined as the number of seconds (to the nearest 10th of a second) between the time that the vignette was presented to the respondent, and the time that the respondent assigned a rating. Some of that time was taken up by the actual time to push the correct key, but that time is impossible to estimate. The OLS regression (estimated without the additive constant), apportions the response time to the different elements in the vignette. The rationale for not estimating the additive constant is that in the absence of elements, the estimated response time is 0. In contrast, for the rating, the additive constant is estimated because in the absence of elements, the additive constant shows the proclivity to be positive to contractor insurance. The model for response time (in seconds) is expressed as: Response Time=k1(A1) + k2(A2) … k16(D4).
Results
We
present the results from the study, looking only at the positive coefficients
for the transformed rating scale. These are the elements and the key subgroups
where the element drives to a rating of YES, operationally defined as a rating
of 4 or 5, previously transformed to 100. Table
2 presents the positive, non-zero elements for total panel, for gender, age
and for three of the four self-declared attitudes about insurance companies.
The fourth answer, not applicable, had only 6 respondents. The negative and 0
coefficients are not shown because they either represent a desire NOT to move
forward and complete the project, or indifference. We will look at the drivers
of desire NOT to move forward below, in Table
4. The additive constant gives a sense of the desire to move forward, to
finish the project. The additive constant is low for gender (36 for males, 30
for females), suggesting a basic disinterest in moving forward. The low
additive constant comes from those age 35-44 and 45+. The older respondents
truly not want to finish the project, whereas the younger respondents do want
to finish the project. Surprising, those age 25-35 show the most interest in
moving forward, to finish the project.
Finally,
as expected, those with a self-declared negative attitude towards insurance
companies show a low additive constant, a low desire to move forward and finish
the project. The elements themselves do not drive the respondents to say that
they would like to finish the project. The only elements which really drive
interest in the combination of contractor and
insurance
are WHO the contractor IS. Viz., D4: Insurance Company pays in full within 7
days.
Table 2: Positive coefficients for elements showing how the element drives interest moving forward with a contractor, along with contractor insurance.
Table 4:Strong-performing coefficients for elements, showing how the element drives interest in STOPPING THE PROJECT The data shows the strong coefficients emerging from a ‘bottom-up analysis’ where the positive coefficients mean stop the Project. Only elements strong in at least one subgroup are shown.
Mind-sets
emerging from the pattern of pattern of responses show a radically different
pattern (see Table 3). Three
mind-sets emerged, two of which are small (MS1 focusing on project; MS2
focusing on the contractor). The third mind-set (focusing on legal and
financial aspects of the job and the contractor relationship constitutes more
than half of the respondents, 55 out of 110. The mind-sets show dramatically
stronger performing elements, which is to be expected since the mind-sets reflect
groups of respondents who think quite differently from each other, based upon
the pattern of their coefficients. The mind-sets differ dramatically in their
basic interest in moving forward, with those respondents in Mind-Set 1 (Project
focused) showing the highest level of interest in moving forward (additive
constant=48). The other two mind-sets, Mind-Set 2 (Contractor focused) and
Mind-Set 3 (Legal/Financial focused) show lowest levels of interest in moving
forward (additive constants 32 and 35, respectively).
It
is in the specific elements where we see the big differences, both in the
nature of the elements with drive ‘moving forward’, and in the magnitude of
those strong-performing elements. Those interested in moving forward, beginning
with the highest basic interest (additive constant=48) are all significantly
positive to the messages about the project, with coefficients between 8 and 10.
In contrast, it is Mind-Set 2, focus on the contractor, the ‘personal link’
which drive the strongest positive response for moving forward. The
coefficients are 8-15, suggest the strong effect of emotions. Finally, those in
Mind-Set 3 (Legal/Finance) react most strongly to the legal and financial
aspects.
When
‘things go wrong’ we get a different picture. Table 4 shows the set of
coefficients we do the reverse analysis, looking for the elements which drive
the respondent to say ‘stop’. The analysis begins with a different recoding.
The ratings at the low end of the scale, 1 and 2, are converted to 100, and the
ratings of 3,4 and 5 are converted to. A small random number is added, and then
the equations are recalculated. The additive constant shows the basic
likelihood of ‘stopping’ without any elements. The coefficient of each element
shows how strong it is as a message to stop the process. We show only the
additive constant and the strong-performing elements. The coefficients are
those corresponding to elements which ‘stop the process’.
The
additive constant, the proclivity to stop the process, is low except for those
respondents who, at the start of the experiment, before the actual evaluation,
declare that they do not trust insurance compaiess. Their additive constant is
53, 14 points higher than the next highest group (Mind-Set 2, focusing on the
contractor). There are two classes of elements which drive to ‘stop’. The first
is the nature of the project, with roof repairs being the least trusted, then
the contractor hired to bring the house into code, and then finally the
contractor hired for remodeling the kitchen. The roof contractor is really the
one least trusted. The second group of elements, which should come as no
surprise, is the failure of the contractor to deliver what has been agreed to.
Surprising, the youngest respondents, age 25-34, are the least likely to
respond that they want to stop the project.
Response
(Consideration) Time-Making a Decision
The
foregoing analyses of responses focus on the conscious evaluation of the different
vignettes. As the data suggests, the results lend themselves to straightforward
interpretation. Even though the speed of the experiment was such that
respondents appear to have rushed through the study, as they were meant to, the
conscious responses suggested that the respondents were actually paying
attention, even though in many of these studies respondents aver quite
vehemently that they were confused, and were simply guessing. That ‘guessing’
certainly does not appear to generate random data.
At a deeper level, however, one can study the response time, the time it takes to read a vignette and rate it. Of course, the time to rate each vignette does not tell us much, just as the rating of a single vignette does not tell us much. Yet, we can use OLS regression to deconstruct the response time into the number of seconds estimated for a person to ‘mentally process’ each element. There will, of course, be some slack time needed to read and to rate, but this will be divided among the individual response times for the elements, those times estimate by OLS regression. The analysis proceeds as before, using as input ALL the data from a particular subgroup (e.g., age, gender, answer to classification question about contractors, mind-sets). We focus here only on the four models, specifically total panel, and the three mind-sets (see Table 5). The response time model is: RT = k1(A1) + k2(A2) … k16(D4). The response time model is expressed in the same way as the binary, except for the absence of the additive constant. The ingoing assumption is that in the absence of elements the response time is 0. The response time is measured to the nearest tenth of second. The coefficients are also presented to the nearest tenth of second, viz., at the level of resolution of the measurement itself, rather than greater resolution (viz., not to the hundredth of a second). Table 5 shows only those response times exceeding 1.3 seconds for the element. There are quite a number of these long response times, especially for Mind-Set 3, focusing on the legal/finance issues, individuals who would be expected to pay attention to the so-called ‘fine print’. Those in Mind-Set 2, paying attention to the contractor, focus on descriptions of the contractor. Those in Mind-Set 1, focusing on the project itself, do not pay deep attention to any of the elements, but rather read them quickly. It is clear that one element needs no thought for driving a judgment, element C1, Project delivered 90 days late. It is clear from the elements of response time, considered in the area of financial topics, that response time or consideration time presents to the researcher an entirely new opportunity to understand the nature of how people think and make decisions in topic areas that are commercial, serious, service-related rather than product related.
Discovering the
Mind-Sets in the Population
During the past sixty years consumer researchers have suggested that the purveyor of products and services might do well by segmenting or dividing the prospects, either by WHO the prospects are (geo-demographics), by hat the prospects BELIEVE (psychographics) or by what the prospects DO (behavior). All three forms of dividing people have their adherents, and their detractors. All three methods, and the dozens of specific procedures in each general method, begin with a general division of the prospective consumers into easy-to-develop groups. Once these groups are created, it is the task of the marketer to know what to say.
This
study, building from the ‘bottom up’, with the specifics and thus granularity
of the topic, suggests a problem with conventional segmentation. The problem is
that the three mind-sets, specific to the topic, divide across conventional
groupings of people, as Table 6 shows. The three mind-sets show similar
distributions in WHO the person is (gender, age) and what the person BELIEVES
(Answer given at the beginning of the study, in the self-classification
portion). One would never guess from Table
6 that the three mind-sets could be so different. These mind-sets comprise
the same type of people, at least from the outside.
Rather
than assuming that people who look similar to each other in terms of gender,
age, or even attitude toward contractors will be similar in the way they
respond to the messages about contractor insurance, a more sensible way might
be to create a small intervention, a set of easy-to-answer questions, the
pattern of responses to which assign a person tone of the three mind-sets. When
this set of questions, the so-called PVI (personal
viewpoint identifier),
is deployed the knowledge about the mind-set membership allows the insurance
salesperson to select the right insurance package for the prospect.
The
interaction becomes more personalized, simply because the insurance salesperson
now knows the ‘insurance-relevant’ mind of the prospect, in a way which is
granular.
Table 6: The division of
respondents into WHO the person is (gender, age), and what the person believes
with respect to insurance companies).
Recently,
author Moskowitz and colleague, Professor Attila Gere, have developed a PVI,
based upon the pattern responses to the elements, and using Monte-Carlo
simulation to identify the best set of relevant elements to use as the six
questions. The PVI enjoys a strong advantage over other methods because the raw
material used to create the PVI is identical to the raw material used to define
the mind-sets. For the PVI presented here, the specific computations were made
from the data summarized in Table 4, showing how each element corresponded to
stopping the project. The tenor of the low side of the scale, stopping the
project, made more sense. Figure 1
shows the PVI for one respondent. The actual link as of this writing (Summer
2020), can be found at https://www.pvi360.com/TypingToolPage.aspx-?projectid=196&userid=2018. Figure 2 shows the feedback for one
respondent. The mind-set to which a respondent is assigned appears as shaded
boxes box. The other two mind-sets appear as unshaded boxes.
Figure 1: The viewpoint
identifier.
Figure 2: The feedback given to the respondent regarding mind-set membership.
Discussion and Conclusions
The
experiment reported here on regarding the specific messages which drive a
prospective customer to purchase insurance covering home repair jobs represents
an intermediate step between the insurance company which designs and sells the
insurance, and the prospective customer who needs to be convinced. As noted
above, much if not a significant proportion of information about what it takes
to convince the prospect to buy comes from the marketing and marketing research
departments of insurance companies. The academic literature focuses on the
patterns of purchase, who purchases, why they purchase, and the financial
aspects of the insurance itself. There are a lot of insurance companies in the
world, and, in turn, a great deal of advertising, advertising testing, and an
entire world of professional
consumer researchers
supporting the effort to sell the advertising. Tools such as focus groups
produce insight into what insurance prospects need, and the language that the
insurance prospects, the customers, actually use to express their need. Following
the early research efforts comes concept tests, and limited roll outs of
insurance plans, combining the insurance company, agents, advertising agencies,
and media specialists.
Mind Genomics occupies a unique position in this mix of expertise, and the mix of different groups. Mind Genomics is an experiment, through which one can understand the specific, granular preferences of prospects in a particular domain, such as home project insurance. What is important to note is that the Mind Genomics effort is to understand the minds of people from the ‘ground up’, for a specific topic (project insurance) rather than to look at the topic from the perspective of theory (e.g., Theory of Reasoned Behavior), or from the perspective of commerce (viz., ‘what works’ in advertising messaging). The ‘larger project’ of Mind Genomics is to assemble the results these studies, and from that assemblage, formulate grounded hypotheses about how the person weighs information and makes decision in the world of commercially-relevant topics.
Acknowledgment
Attila Gere thanks the support of Premium Postdoctoral Research Program of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences.
References
- Fels M. Mental accounting, access
motives, and overinsurance (2020) Scand J Econ 122: 675-701. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjoe.12336
- French CC. Revisiting
Construction Defects as Occurrences under CGL Insurance Policies (2016)
University of Pennsylvania Journal of Business Law 19: 101.
- Brockett PL, Golden LL and Betak
J. Different market methods for transferring financial risks in construction
(2019) Risk Management in Construction Projects. https://doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.84748
- Brogan E, McConnell W and Clevenger
MC. Emerging patterns in construction defect litigation: Survey of construction
cases (2018) J Legal Affairs and Dispute Resolution in Engineering and
Construction 10: 03718003. https://doi.org/10.1061/(asce)la.1943-4170.0000277
- Stover MA. A Guide to Builder's
Risk Insurance (2018) Tort Trial Insurance Practice Law J 53: 819-847.
- Celuch K, Taylor SA and Goodwin
S. Understanding insurance salesperson internet information management intentions:
A test of competing models (2004) J Insurance Issues pp. 22-40.
- Fletcher KP and Hastings WJ.
Consumer choice: a study of insurance buying intention, attitudes and beliefs
(1984) The Service Industries J 4: 174-188. https://doi.org/10.1080/02642068400000034
- Hastings WJ and Fletcher KP. The
relevance of the Fishbein model to insurance buying (1983) Service Industries J
3: 296-307. https://doi.org/10.1080/02642068300000037
- Moskowitz HR. ‘Mind genomics’:
The experimental, inductive science of the ordinary, and its application to
aspects of food and feeding (2012) Physiol behav 107: 606-613. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.009
- Moskowitz HR, Gofman A, Beckley J
and Ashman H. Founding a new science: Mind genomics (2006) J sensory studies
21: 266-307. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-459x.2004.00066.x
- Moskowitz HR and Gofman A.
Selling blue elephants: How to make great products that people want before they
even know they want them (2007) Pearson Education 25: 637-638. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5885.2008.00327_3.x
- Schwartz B and Ward A. Doing
better but feeling worse: The paradox of choice (2004) Positive psychol
practice 86-104. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939338.ch6
- Gofman A and Moskowitz H.
Isomorphic permuted experimental designs and their application in conjoint
analysis (2010) J Sensory Studies 25: 127-145. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-459x.2009.00258.x
*Corresponding author
Howard Moskowitz, MindCart AI, Inc., White Plains, New York, USA, E-mail: mjihrm@gmail.com
Citation
Moskowitz H
and Gere H. Selling to the mind of the insurance prospect: A mind genomics
cartography of insurance for home project contracts (2020) Edelweiss Psyi
OpenAccess 4: 22-28.
Keywords
Mind genomics, Cartography, Insurance